Featured Research
from universities, journals, and other organizations
How we form habits, change existing ones
Date:
August 8, 2014
Source:
Society for Personality and Social Psychology
Summary:
About 40 percent of
people's daily activities are performed each day in almost the same
situations, studies show. Habits emerge through associative learning.
'We find patterns of behavior that allow us to reach goals. We repeat
what works, and when actions are repeated in a stable context, we form
associations between cues and response,' a researcher explains.
Much
of our daily lives are taken up by habits that we've formed over our
lifetime. An important characteristic of a habit is that it's
automatic-- we don't always recognize habits in our own behavior.
Studies show that about 40 percent of people's daily activities are
performed each day in almost the same situations. Habits emerge through
associative learning. "We find patterns of behavior that allow us to
reach goals. We repeat what works, and when actions are repeated in a
stable context, we form associations between cues and response," Wendy
Wood explains in her session at the American Psychological Association's
122nd Annual Convention.
What are habits?
Wood calls attention to the neurology of habits, and how they have a recognizable neural signature. When you are learning a response you engage your associative basal ganglia, which involves the prefrontal cortex and supports working memory so you can make decisions. As you repeat the behavior in the same context, the information is reorganized in your brain. It shifts to the sensory motor loop that supports representations of cue response associations, and no longer retains information on the goal or outcome. This shift from goal directed to context cue response helps to explain why our habits are rigid behaviors.
There is a dual mind at play, Wood explains. When our intentional mind is engaged, we act in ways that meet an outcome we desire and typically we're aware of our intentions. Intentions can change quickly because we can make conscious decisions about what we want to do in the future that may be different from the past. However, when the habitual mind is engaged, our habits function largely outside of awareness. We can't easily articulate how we do our habits or why we do them, and they change slowly through repeated experience. "Our minds don't always integrate in the best way possible. Even when you know the right answer, you can't make yourself change the habitual behavior," Wood says.
Participants in a study were asked to taste popcorn, and as expected, fresh popcorn was preferable to stale. But when participants were given popcorn in a movie theater, people who have a habit of eating popcorn at the movies ate just as much stale popcorn as participants in the fresh popcorn group. "The thoughtful intentional mind is easily derailed and people tend to fall back on habitual behaviors. Forty percent of the time we're not thinking about what we're doing," Wood interjects. "Habits allow us to focus on other things…Willpower is a limited resource, and when it runs out you fall back on habits."
How can we change our habits?
Public service announcements, educational programs, community workshops, and weight-loss programs are all geared toward improving your day-to-day habits. But are they really effective? These standard interventions are very successful at increasing motivation and desire. You will almost always leave feeling like you can change and that you want to change. The programs give you knowledge and goal-setting strategies for implementation, but these programs only address the intentional mind.
In a study on the "Take 5" program, 35 percent of people polled came away believing they should eat 5 fruits and vegetables a day. Looking at that result, it appears that the national program was effective at teaching people that it's important to have 5 servings of fruits and vegetables every day. But the data changes when you ask what people are actually eating. Only 11 percent of people reported that they met this goal. The program changed people's intentions, but it did not overrule habitual behavior.
According to Wood, there are three main principles to consider when effectively changing habitual behavior. First, you must derail existing habits and create a window of opportunity to act on new intentions. Someone who moves to a new city or changes jobs has the perfect scenario to disrupt old cues and create new habits. When the cues for existing habits are removed, it's easier to form a new behavior. If you can't alter your entire environment by switching cities-- make small changes. For instance, if weight-loss or healthy eating is your goal, try moving unhealthy foods to a top shelf out of reach, or to the back of the freezer instead of in front.
The second principle is remembering that repetition is key. Studies have shown it can take anywhere from 15 days to 254 days to truly form a new habit. "There's no easy formula for how long it takes," Wood says. Lastly, there must be stable context cues available in order to trigger a new pattern. "It's easier to maintain the behavior if it's repeated in a specific context," Wood emphasizes. Flossing after you brush your teeth allows the act of brushing to be the cue to remember to floss. Reversing the two behaviors is not as successful at creating a new flossing habit. Having an initial cue is a crucial component.
Wood calls attention to the neurology of habits, and how they have a recognizable neural signature. When you are learning a response you engage your associative basal ganglia, which involves the prefrontal cortex and supports working memory so you can make decisions. As you repeat the behavior in the same context, the information is reorganized in your brain. It shifts to the sensory motor loop that supports representations of cue response associations, and no longer retains information on the goal or outcome. This shift from goal directed to context cue response helps to explain why our habits are rigid behaviors.
There is a dual mind at play, Wood explains. When our intentional mind is engaged, we act in ways that meet an outcome we desire and typically we're aware of our intentions. Intentions can change quickly because we can make conscious decisions about what we want to do in the future that may be different from the past. However, when the habitual mind is engaged, our habits function largely outside of awareness. We can't easily articulate how we do our habits or why we do them, and they change slowly through repeated experience. "Our minds don't always integrate in the best way possible. Even when you know the right answer, you can't make yourself change the habitual behavior," Wood says.
Participants in a study were asked to taste popcorn, and as expected, fresh popcorn was preferable to stale. But when participants were given popcorn in a movie theater, people who have a habit of eating popcorn at the movies ate just as much stale popcorn as participants in the fresh popcorn group. "The thoughtful intentional mind is easily derailed and people tend to fall back on habitual behaviors. Forty percent of the time we're not thinking about what we're doing," Wood interjects. "Habits allow us to focus on other things…Willpower is a limited resource, and when it runs out you fall back on habits."
How can we change our habits?
Public service announcements, educational programs, community workshops, and weight-loss programs are all geared toward improving your day-to-day habits. But are they really effective? These standard interventions are very successful at increasing motivation and desire. You will almost always leave feeling like you can change and that you want to change. The programs give you knowledge and goal-setting strategies for implementation, but these programs only address the intentional mind.
In a study on the "Take 5" program, 35 percent of people polled came away believing they should eat 5 fruits and vegetables a day. Looking at that result, it appears that the national program was effective at teaching people that it's important to have 5 servings of fruits and vegetables every day. But the data changes when you ask what people are actually eating. Only 11 percent of people reported that they met this goal. The program changed people's intentions, but it did not overrule habitual behavior.
According to Wood, there are three main principles to consider when effectively changing habitual behavior. First, you must derail existing habits and create a window of opportunity to act on new intentions. Someone who moves to a new city or changes jobs has the perfect scenario to disrupt old cues and create new habits. When the cues for existing habits are removed, it's easier to form a new behavior. If you can't alter your entire environment by switching cities-- make small changes. For instance, if weight-loss or healthy eating is your goal, try moving unhealthy foods to a top shelf out of reach, or to the back of the freezer instead of in front.
The second principle is remembering that repetition is key. Studies have shown it can take anywhere from 15 days to 254 days to truly form a new habit. "There's no easy formula for how long it takes," Wood says. Lastly, there must be stable context cues available in order to trigger a new pattern. "It's easier to maintain the behavior if it's repeated in a specific context," Wood emphasizes. Flossing after you brush your teeth allows the act of brushing to be the cue to remember to floss. Reversing the two behaviors is not as successful at creating a new flossing habit. Having an initial cue is a crucial component.
Story Source:
The above story is based on materials provided by Society for Personality and Social Psychology. Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.
The above story is based on materials provided by Society for Personality and Social Psychology. Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.
Journal References:
- D. T. Neal, W. Wood, M. Wu, D. Kurlander. The Pull of the Past: When Do Habits Persist Despite Conflict With Motives? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2011; 37 (11): 1428 DOI: 10.1177/0146167211419863
- Sarah Stark Casagrande, Youfa Wang, Cheryl Anderson, Tiffany L. Gary. Have Americans Increased Their Fruit and Vegetable Intake?The Trends Between 1988 and 2002. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 2007; 32 (4): 257 DOI: 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.12.002
Cite This Page:
Share This
More Mind & Brain News
Friday, August 8, 2014
Featured Research
from universities, journals, and other organizations
How We Form Habits, Change Existing Ones
Aug. 8, 2014 — About 40 percent of
people's daily activities are performed each day in almost the same
situations, studies show. Habits emerge through associative learning.
'We find patterns of behavior that allow ... full story
- How We Form Habits, Change Existing Ones
- Mystery of Brain Cell Growth Unraveled
- Meaning of 'Diversity' May Depend On Your Race
- Lipids Boost the Brain, Study Finds
- Work-Related Stress a Risk Factor for Diabetes
- Stem Cells Show Promise for Stroke: Pilot Study
- Constitutional Rights and Youth Interrogations
- Musical Training Boosts Academic Achievement?
- Pervasive Sleep Problems in Astronauts
- Slowing Brain Functions: Risk of Stroke, Death
newer top stories | older top stories
Featured Videos
from AP, Reuters, AFP, and other news services
Save/Print:
Share:
Breaking News:
Trending Topics
from the past week
Health & Medicine
- Breast Cancer
- Stem Cells
- Diabetes
- Hormone Disorders
- Medical Education and Training
- Pregnancy and Childbirth
- Skin Care
- Bone and Spine
Mind & Brain
- Stress
- Gender Difference
- Perception
- Racial Issues
- Anger Management
- Anxiety
- Autism
- Brain-Computer Interfaces
Living & Well
In Other News
... from NewsDaily.com
Science News
- Scientists retract narcolepsy study linked to GSK vaccine
- Robotic helpers? Scientists tout cheap robot that assembles itself
- Drone test flights start in NY for agricultural research: FAA
- Spacecraft Rosetta catches up to comet after 10-year chase
- Butt batteries: Scientists store energy in used cigarette filters
Health News
- WHO declares Ebola an international health emergency
- Atlanta confronts tuberculosis outbreak in homeless shelters
- Kids at high-performing charter schools make better health decisions: study
- Dentists miss chances to urge patients to quit smoking
- Farmers market vouchers may improve access to healthy food
Environment News
- Hurricane, tropical storm roll toward Hawaii
- Happy birthday, Smokey Bear! Forest firefighting symbol turns 70
- Wyoming cave dig unearths bones of ancient horses, cheetahs and bison
- Growing wildfire threatens hundreds more homes in Oregon
- Hawaiian refineries running as Iselle brings wind, rain
Technology News
- Gemalto to buy U.S. data protection firm SafeNet for $890 million
- Google settles U.S. shareholder lawsuit over online drug ads
- Judge rejects $324.5 million settlement over Apple, Google hiring
- After Verizon, U.S. FCC quizzing other carriers on data management
- Hacking group wants to play nice with automakers
Save/Print:
Share:
Free Subscriptions
Get the latest science news with ScienceDaily's free email
newsletters, updated daily and weekly. Or view hourly updated newsfeeds
in your RSS reader:
Get Social & Mobile
Keep up to date with the latest news from ScienceDaily via social networks and mobile apps:
Have Feedback?
Tell us what you think of ScienceDaily -- we welcome both positive
and negative comments. Have any problems using the site? Questions?
No comments:
Post a Comment
Please leave a comment-- or suggestions, particularly of topics and places you'd like to see covered